toggle visibility Search & Display Options

Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print
Barker, S. C. (2003). The Australian paralysis tick may be the missing link in the transmission of Hendra virus from bats to horses to humans. Med Hypotheses, 60(4), 481–483.
toggle visibility
Church, R. M. (1997). Quantitative models of animal learning and cognition. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process, 23(4), 379–389.
toggle visibility
Flack, J. C., Krakauer, D. C., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2005). Robustness mechanisms in primate societies: a perturbation study. Proc Biol Sci, 272(1568), 1091–1099.
toggle visibility
Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I. J. Theor. Biol., 7(1and 2), 1–52.
toggle visibility
Johnstone, R. A. (2001). Eavesdropping and animal conflict. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 98(16), 9177–9180.
toggle visibility
Lee, R. D. (2003). Rethinking the evolutionary theory of aging: transfers, not births, shape senescence in social species. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 100(16), 9637–9642.
toggle visibility
Pepperberg, I. M. (2002). The value of the Piagetian framework for comparative cognitive studies. Anim. Cogn., 5(3), 177–182.
toggle visibility
Shettleworth, S. J. (2005). Taking the best for learning. Behav. Process., 69(2), 147–9; author reply 159–63.
toggle visibility
Sundaresan, S. R., Fischhoff, I. R., Dushoff, J., & Rubenstein, D. I. (2007). Network metrics reveal differences in social organization between two fission-fusion species, Grevy's zebra and onager. Oecologia, 151(1), 140–149.
toggle visibility
Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print