toggle visibility Search & Display Options

Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print
Johnson-Pynn, J., & Fragaszy, D. M. (2001). Do apes and monkeys rely upon conceptual reversibility? Anim. Cogn., 4(3), 315–324.
toggle visibility
Johnstone, R. A. (2001). Eavesdropping and animal conflict. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 98(16), 9177–9180.
toggle visibility
Kasuya, E. (2001). Mann-Whitney U test when variances are unequal. Anim. Behav., 61(6), 1247–1249.
toggle visibility
Kelly, D. M., & Spetch, M. L. (2001). Pigeons encode relative geometry. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process, 27(4), 417–422.
toggle visibility
Kimura, R. (2001). Volatile substances in feces, urine and urine-marked feces of feral horses. Can. J. Anim. Sci., 81(3), 411–420.
toggle visibility
Kirkwood, J. K., & Hubrecht, R. (2001). Animal Consciousness, Cognition and Welfare. Animal Welfare, 10, 5–17.
toggle visibility
Koba, Y., & Tanida, H. (2001). How do miniature pigs discriminate between people?: Discrimination between people wearing coveralls of the same colour. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci., 73(1), 45–58.
toggle visibility
Kudo, H., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2001). Neocortex size and social network size in primates. Anim. Behav., 62(4), 711–722.
toggle visibility
Kutsukake, N., & Castles, D. L. (2001). Reconciliation and variation in post-conflict stress in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata fuscata): testing the integrated hypothesis. Anim. Cogn., 4(3), 259–268.
toggle visibility
Lea, S. E. G. (2001). Anticipation and Memory as Criteria for Special Welfare Consideration. Animal Welfare, 10, 195–208.
toggle visibility
Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print