toggle visibility Search & Display Options

Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print
Herrmann, E., Melis, A. P., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Apes' use of iconic cues in the object-choice task. Anim. Cogn., 9(2), 118–130.
toggle visibility
Paukner, A., Anderson, J. R., & Fujita, K. (2006). Redundant food searches by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): a failure of metacognition? Anim. Cogn., 9(2), 110–117.
toggle visibility
Palmer, M. E., Calve, M. R., & Adamo, S. A. (2006). Response of female cuttlefish Sepia officinalis (Cephalopoda) to mirrors and conspecifics: evidence for signaling in female cuttlefish. Anim. Cogn., 9(2), 151–155.
toggle visibility
Schmoldt, A., Benthe, H. F., & Haberland, G. (1975). Digitoxin metabolism by rat liver microsomes. Biochem Pharmacol, 24(17), 1639–1641.
toggle visibility
Jallon, J. M., Risler, Y., & Iwatsubo, M. (1975). Beef liver L-Glutamate dehydrogenase mechanism: presteady state study of the catalytic reduction of 2.oxoglutarate by NADPH. Biochem Biophys Res Commun, 67(4), 1527–1536.
toggle visibility
Rodier, F. (1976). [Spectral properties of porcine plasminogen: study of the acidic transition (author's transl)]. Eur J Biochem, 63(2), 553–562.
toggle visibility
Zentall, T. R. (2001). The case for a cognitive approach to animal learning and behavior. Behav Processes, 54(1-3), 65–78.
toggle visibility
Bloom, P. (2004). Behavior. Can a dog learn a word? Science, 304(5677), 1605–1606.
toggle visibility
NICOLA J. ROONEY & JOHN W. S. BRADSHAW. (2005). Social cognition in the domestic dog: behaviour of spectators towards participants in interspecific games. Anim. Behav., 72(2), 343–352.
toggle visibility
Houpt, K. A. (2006). Why horse behaviour is important to the equine clinician. Equine Vet J, 38(5), 386–387.
toggle visibility
Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print