toggle visibility Search & Display Options

Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print
Dyer, F. C. (1998). Spatial Cognition: Lessons from Central-place Foraging Insects. In Russell P. Balda, Irene M. Pepperberg, & Alan C. Kamil (Eds.), Animal Cognition in Nature (pp. 119–154). London: Academic Press.
toggle visibility
Smith, W. J. (1998). Cognitive Implications of an Information-sharing Model of Animal Communication. In Russell P. Balda, Irene M. Pepperberg, & Alan C. Kamil (Eds.), Animal Cognition in Nature (pp. 227–243). London: Academic Press.
toggle visibility
Beer, C. G. (1998). Varying Views of Animal and Human Cognition. In Russell P. Balda, Irene M. Pepperberg, & Alan C. Kamil (Eds.), Animal Cognition in Nature (pp. 435–456). London: Academic Press.
toggle visibility
Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, & Frith U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37.
toggle visibility
Wimmer H, & Perner J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103.
toggle visibility
Hauser MD. (1997). Artifactual kinds and functional design features: what a primate understands without language. Cognition, 64, 285.
toggle visibility
Premack D, & Premack AJ. (1994). Levels of causal understanding in chimpanzees and children. Cognition, 50, 347.
toggle visibility
Santos LR, Hauser MD, & Spelke ES. (2001). Recognition and categorization of biologically significant objects by rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta): the domain of food. Cognition, 82, 127.
toggle visibility
Cook, R. G., Shaw, R., & Blaisdell, A. P. (2001). Dynamic object perception by pigeons: discrimination of action in video presentations. Anim. Cogn., 4(3), 137–146.
toggle visibility
Forkman, B. (2000). Domestic hens have declarative representations. Anim. Cogn., 3(3), 135–137.
toggle visibility
Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print