toggle visibility Search & Display Options

Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print
Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (2004). The role of humans in the cognitive development of apes revisited. Anim. Cogn., 7(4), 213–215.
toggle visibility
Cheng, K. (2004). K.J. Jeffery (ed) The neurobiology of spatial behaviourOxford University Press, Oxford, 2003. Anim. Cogn., .
toggle visibility
Leighty, K. A., & Fragaszy, D. M. (2003). Primates in cyberspace: using interactive computer tasks to study perception and action in nonhuman animals. Anim. Cogn., 6(3), 137–139.
toggle visibility
Hare, J. F., Sealy, S. G., Underwood, T. J., Ellison, K. S., & Stewart, R. L. M. (2003). Evidence of self-referent phenotype matching revisited: airing out the armpit effect. Anim. Cogn., 6(1), 65–68.
toggle visibility
Mateo, J. M., & Johnston, R. E. (2003). Kin recognition by self-referent phenotype matching: weighing the evidence. Anim. Cogn., 6(1), 73–76.
toggle visibility
Hauber, M. E., & Sherman, P. W. (2003). Designing and interpreting experimental tests of self-referent phenotype matching. Anim. Cogn., 6(1), 69–71.
toggle visibility
Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, & Frith U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37.
toggle visibility
Wimmer H, & Perner J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103.
toggle visibility
Hauser MD. (1997). Artifactual kinds and functional design features: what a primate understands without language. Cognition, 64, 285.
toggle visibility
Premack D, & Premack AJ. (1994). Levels of causal understanding in chimpanzees and children. Cognition, 50, 347.
toggle visibility
Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print