toggle visibility Search & Display Options

Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print
Sovrano, V. A., Rainoldi, C., Bisazza, A., & Vallortigara, G. (1999). Roots of brain specializations: preferential left-eye use during mirror-image inspection in six species of teleost fish. Behav. Brain. Res., 106(1-2), 175–180.
toggle visibility
Ratcliffe, J. M., Fenton, M. B., & Shettleworth, S. J. (2006). Behavioral flexibility positively correlated with relative brain volume in predatory bats. Brain Behav Evol, 67(3), 165–176.
toggle visibility
Jackson, R. R., Pollard, S. D., & Cerveira, A. M. (2002). Opportunistic use of cognitive smokescreens by araneophagic jumping spiders. Anim. Cogn., 5(3), 147–157.
toggle visibility
Christensen, J. W., & Rundgren, M. (2008). Predator odour per se does not frighten domestic horses. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci., 112(1-2), 136–145.
toggle visibility
Griffin, A. S. (2008). Social learning in Indian mynahs, Acridotheres tristis: the role of distress calls. Anim. Behav., 75(1), 79–89.
toggle visibility
Burke, D., Cieplucha, C., Cass, J., Russell, F., & Fry, G. (2002). Win-shift and win-stay learning in the short-beaked echidna (Tachyglossus aculeatus). Anim. Cogn., 5(2), 79–84.
toggle visibility
Heinrich, B., & Bugnyar, T. (2007). Just how smart are ravens? Sci Am, 296(4), 64–71.
toggle visibility
Punzo, F., & Ludwig, L. (2002). Contact with maternal parent and siblings affects hunting behavior, learning, and central nervous system development in spiderlings of Hogna carolinensis (Araeneae: Lycosidae). Anim. Cogn., 5(2), 63–70.
toggle visibility
Hampton, R. R., Sherry, D. F., Shettleworth, S. J., Khurgel, M., & Ivy, G. (1995). Hippocampal volume and food-storing behavior are related in parids. Brain Behav Evol, 45(1), 54–61.
toggle visibility
Krama, T. [1], & Krams, I. [2]. (2005). Cost of mobbing call to breeding pied flycatcher, Ficedula hypoleuca. Behav. Ecol., 16, 37–40.
toggle visibility
Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print