toggle visibility Search & Display Options

Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print
Boice, R. (1981). Behavioral comparability of wild and domesticated rats. Behav Genet, 11(5), 545–553.
toggle visibility
Watanabe, S., & Troje, N. F. (2006). Towards a “virtual pigeon”: a new technique for investigating avian social perception. Anim. Cogn., 9(4), 271–279.
toggle visibility
Wallace, D. G., Hamilton, D. A., & Whishaw, I. Q. (2006). Movement characteristics support a role for dead reckoning in organizing exploratory behavior. Anim. Cogn., 9(3), 219–228.
toggle visibility
Rumiantsev, S. N. (1973). [Biological function of Clostridium tetani toxin (ecological and evolutionary aspects)]. Zh Evol Biokhim Fiziol, 9(5), 474–480.
toggle visibility
Tempelis, C. H., & Nelson, R. L. (1971). Blood-feeding patterns of midges of the Culicoides variipennis complex in Kern County, California. J Med Entomol, 8(5), 532–534.
toggle visibility
Boray, J. C. (1969). Experimental fascioliasis in Australia. Adv Parasitol, 7, 95–210.
toggle visibility
Toro, J. M., Trobalon, J. B., & Sebastian-Galles, N. (2003). The use of prosodic cues in language discrimination tasks by rats. Anim. Cogn., 6(2), 131–136.
toggle visibility
Cohen, J., Pardy, S., Solway, H., & Graham, H. (2003). Chunking versus foraging search patterns by rats in the hierarchically baited radial maze. Anim. Cogn., 6(2), 93–104.
toggle visibility
Weatherly, J. N., Arthur, E. I. L., & Tischart, L. M. (2003). Altering “motivational” variables alters induction produced by upcoming food-pellet reinforcement. Anim. Cogn., 6(1), 17–26.
toggle visibility
Ray, E. D., & Heyes, C. M. (2002). Do rats in a two-action test encode movement egocentrically or allocentrically? Anim. Cogn., 5(4), 245–252.
toggle visibility
Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print