toggle visibility Search & Display Options

Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print
Sovrano, V. A., Bisazza, A., & Vallortigara, G. (2007). How fish do geometry in large and in small spaces. Anim. Cogn., 10(1), 47–54.
toggle visibility
Sovrano, V., & Bisazza, A. (2008). Recognition of partly occluded objects by fish. Anim. Cogn., 11(1), 1435–9448.
toggle visibility
Sousa, C., Okamoto, S., & Matsuzawa, T. (2003). Behavioural development in a matching-to-sample task and token use by an infant chimpanzee reared by his mother. Anim. Cogn., 6(4), 259–267.
toggle visibility
Sousa, C., & Matsuzawa, T. (2001). The use of tokens as rewards and tools by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Anim. Cogn., 4(3), 213–221.
toggle visibility
Sommer, V., Lowe, A., & Dietrich, T. (2016). Not eating like a pig: European wild boar wash their food. Anim. Cogn., 19(1), 245–249.
toggle visibility
Soler, M., & Soler, J. J. (1999). Innate versus learned recognition of conspecifics in great spotted cuckoos Clamator glandarius. Anim. Cogn., 2(2), 97–102.
toggle visibility
Snowdon, C. T. (2001). Social processes in communication and cognition in callitrichid monkeys: a review. Anim. Cogn., 4(3), 247–257.
toggle visibility
Smith, B., & Litchfield, C. (2010). Dingoes (Canis dingo) can use human social cues to locate hidden food. Anim. Cogn., 13(2), 367–376.
toggle visibility
Slobodchikoff, C., Paseka, A., & Verdolin, J. (2009). Prairie dog alarm calls encode labels about predator colors. Anim. Cogn., 12(3), 435–439.
toggle visibility
Shuster, G., & Sherman, P. W. (1998). Tool use by naked mole-rats. Anim. Cogn., 1(1), 71–74.
toggle visibility
Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print