toggle visibility Search & Display Options

Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print
Yeon, S. C. (2012). Acoustic communication in the domestic horse (Equus caballus). Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research, 7(3), 179–185.
toggle visibility
Leiner, L., & Fendt, M. (2011). Behavioural fear and heart rate responses of horses after exposure to novel objects: Effects of habituation. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci., 131(3-4), 104–109.
toggle visibility
Pickerel, T. M., Crowell-Davis, S. L., Caudle, A. B., & Estep, D. Q. (1993). Sexual preference of mares (Equus caballus) for individual stallions. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci., 38(1), 1–13.
toggle visibility
Hostetter, A. B., Cantero, M., & Hopkins, W. D. (2001). Differential use of vocal and gestural communication by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in response to the attentional status of a human (Homo sapiens). J. Comp. Psychol., 115(4), 337–343.
toggle visibility
Andrew, R. J. (1974). Changes in visual responsiveness following intercollicular lesions and their effects on avoidance and attack. Brain Behav Evol, 10(4-5), 400–424.
toggle visibility
Thornton, A., & McAuliffe, K. (2006). Teaching in wild meerkats. Science, 313(5784), 227–229.
toggle visibility
Dunbar, R. (2003). Evolution of the social brain. Science, 302(5648), 1160–1161.
toggle visibility
Peake, T. M., Terry, A. M. R., McGregor, P. K., & Dabelsteen, T. (2002). Do great tits assess rivals by combining direct experience with information gathered by eavesdropping? Proc Biol Sci, 269(1503), 1925–1929.
toggle visibility
Peake, T. M., Terry, A. M., McGregor, P. K., & Dabelsteen, T. (2001). Male great tits eavesdrop on simulated male-to-male vocal interactions. Proc Biol Sci, 268(1472), 1183–1187.
toggle visibility
Arnold, K., & Zuberbuhler, K. (2006). Language evolution: semantic combinations in primate calls. Nature, 441(7091), 303.
toggle visibility
Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print