toggle visibility Search & Display Options

Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print
Gould, J. L. (2004). Thinking about thinking: how Donald R. Griffin (1915-2003) remade animal behavior. Anim. Cogn., 7(1), 1–4.
toggle visibility
Hsu, Y., & Wolf, L. L. (1999). The winner and loser effect: integrating multiple experiences. Anim. Behav., 57(4), 903–910.
toggle visibility
Breummer, F. (1967). The wild horses of Sable Island. Animals, 10, 14–17.
toggle visibility
Pepperberg, I. M. (2002). The value of the Piagetian framework for comparative cognitive studies. Anim. Cogn., 5(3), 177–182.
toggle visibility
Nelson, X. J., & Fijn, N. (2013). The use of visual media as a tool for investigating animal behaviour. Animal Behaviour, 85(3), 525–536.
toggle visibility
Sousa, C., & Matsuzawa, T. (2001). The use of tokens as rewards and tools by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Anim. Cogn., 4(3), 213–221.
toggle visibility
Toro, J. M., Trobalon, J. B., & Sebastian-Galles, N. (2003). The use of prosodic cues in language discrimination tasks by rats. Anim. Cogn., 6(2), 131–136.
toggle visibility
Chilton, N. B. (2004). The use of nuclear ribosomal DNA markers for the identification of bursate nematodes (order Strongylida) and for the diagnosis of infections. Anim Health Res Rev, 5(2), 173–187.
toggle visibility
McAfee L.M., Mills D.S., & Cooper J.J. (2002). The use of mirrors for the control of stereotypic weaving behaviour in the stabled horse. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci., 78, 159–173.
toggle visibility
Proops, L., Walton, M., & McComb, K. (2010). The use of human-given cues by domestic horses, Equus caballus, during an object choice task. Anim. Behav., 79(6), 1205–1209.
toggle visibility
Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print