| 
Citations
 | 
   web
Takimoto, A., Kuroshima, H., & Fujita, K. (2010). Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) are sensitive to others’ reward: an experimental analysis of food-choice for conspecifics. Anim. Cogn., 13(2), 249–261.
toggle visibility
Pelé, M., & Sueur, C. (2013). Decision-making theories: linking the disparate research areas of individual and collective cognition. Animal Cognition, 16(4), 543–556.
toggle visibility
Chalmeau, R., & Gallo, A. (1993). Social constraints determine what is learned in the chimpanzee. Behav. Process., 28(3), 173–179.
toggle visibility
Mejdell, C. M., Buvik, T., Jørgensen, G. H. M., & Bøe, K. E. (2016). Horses can learn to use symbols to communicate their preferences. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci., 184, 66–73.
toggle visibility
Mejdell, C. M., Buvik, T., Jørgensen, G. H. M., & Bøe, K. E. (2016). Horses can learn to use symbols to communicate their preferences. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci., 184, 66–73.
toggle visibility
Mejdell, C. M., Buvik, T., Jørgensen, G. H. M., & Bøe, K. E. (2016). Horses can learn to use symbols to communicate their preferences. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci., 184, 66–73.
toggle visibility
Rubenstein, D. I., & Hack, M. A. (1992). Horse signals: The sounds and scents of fury. Evol. Ecol., 6(3), 254–260.
toggle visibility
Wilson, D. A., & Stevenson, R. J. (2003). The fundamental role of memory in olfactory perception. Trends. Neurosci., 26(5), 243–247.
toggle visibility
Keverne, E. B. (1995). Olfactory learning. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol., 5(4), 482–488.
toggle visibility
Kimura, R. (2001). Volatile substances in feces, urine and urine-marked feces of feral horses. Can. J. Anim. Sci., 81(3), 411–420.
toggle visibility