toggle visibility Search & Display Options

Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print
Custance DM, Whiten A, & Bard KA. (1995). Can young chimpanzees imitate arbitrary actions? Hayes and Hayes (1952) revisited. Behavior, 132, 839.
toggle visibility
Saleh, N., & Chittka, L. (2006). The importance of experience in the interpretation of conspecific chemical signals. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 61(2), 215–220.
toggle visibility
Dugatkin, L. A., & Alfieri, M. (1991). Guppies and the TIT FOR TAT strategy: preference based on past interaction. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 28(4), 243–246.
toggle visibility
Sluyter F., Arseneault L., Moffitt T.E., Veenema A.H., de Boer S., & Koolhaas J.M. (2003). Toward an Animal Model for Antisocial Behavior: Parallels Between Mice and Humans: Aggression. Behavior Genetics, 33, 563–574.
toggle visibility
Kurtzman H.S., Church R.M., & Crystal J.D. (2002). Data archiving for animal cognition research: Report of an NIMH workshop. Animal Learning & Behavior, 30, 405–412.
toggle visibility
McGreevy, P. D. (2004).
toggle visibility
Waring, G. H. (2003).
toggle visibility
Grzimek, B. (1968). On the Psychology of the Horse. Man and Animal: Studies in Behavior, , 37–46.
toggle visibility
Boitani, L. (1982). Patterns of homesites attendance in two Minnesota wolf packs. In F. H. Harrington, & P. C. Paquet (Eds.), Wolves of the World: Perspectives of Behavior, Ecology and Conservation. New York: Noyes, Park Ridge.
toggle visibility
Zentall, T. R. (2001). The case for a cognitive approach to animal learning and behavior. Behav Processes, 54(1-3), 65–78.
toggle visibility
Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print