| 
Citations
 | 
   web
Ward, C., & Smuts, B. B. (2007). Quantity-based judgments in the domestic dog (Canis lupus familiaris). Anim. Cogn., 10(1), 71–80.
toggle visibility
Vlasak, A. N. (2006). Global and local spatial landmarks: their role during foraging by Columbian ground squirrels (Spermophilus columbianus). Anim. Cogn., 9(1), 71–80.
toggle visibility
Chappell, J., & Kacelnik, A. (2002). Tool selectivity in a non-primate, the New Caledonian crow (Corvus moneduloides). Anim. Cogn., 5(2), 71–78.
toggle visibility
Shuster, G., & Sherman, P. W. (1998). Tool use by naked mole-rats. Anim. Cogn., 1(1), 71–74.
toggle visibility
Bugnyar, T., & Kotrschal, K. (2004). Leading a conspecific away from food in ravens ( Corvus corax)? Anim. Cogn., 7(2), 69–76.
toggle visibility
Hauber, M. E., & Sherman, P. W. (2003). Designing and interpreting experimental tests of self-referent phenotype matching. Anim. Cogn., 6(1), 69–71.
toggle visibility
Potì, P. (2000). Aspects of spatial cognition in capuchins (Cebus apella): frames of reference and scale of space. Anim. Cogn., 3(2), 69–77.
toggle visibility
Webster, S., & Fiorito, G. (2001). Socially guided behaviour in non-insect invertebrates. Anim. Cogn., 4(2), 69–79.
toggle visibility
Helton, W. S. (2005). Animal expertise, conscious or not. Anim. Cogn., 8(2), 67–74.
toggle visibility
Hare, J. F., Sealy, S. G., Underwood, T. J., Ellison, K. S., & Stewart, R. L. M. (2003). Evidence of self-referent phenotype matching revisited: airing out the armpit effect. Anim. Cogn., 6(1), 65–68.
toggle visibility