toggle visibility Search & Display Options

Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print
Mithen, S. (2007). Did farming arise from a misapplication of social intelligence? Phil. Trans. Biol. Sci., 362(1480), 705–718.
toggle visibility
Byrne, R. W. (1993). Do larger brains mean greater intelligence? Behav. Brain Sci., 16(4), 696–697.
toggle visibility
Griffiths, S. W., Brockmark, S., Höjesjö, J., & Johnsson, J. I. (2004). Coping with divided attention: the advantage of familiarity. Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci., 271(1540), 695–699.
toggle visibility
Houpt, K., Marrow, M., & Seeliger, M. (2000). A preliminary study of the effect of music on equine behavior. Journal of Equine Veterinary Science, 20(11), 691–737.
toggle visibility
Rosati, A. G. (2017). Foraging Cognition: Reviving the Ecological Intelligence Hypothesis. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(9), 691–702.
toggle visibility
Houpt, K., Marrow, M., & Seeliger, M. (2000). A preliminary study of the effect of music on equine behavior. Journal of Equine Veterinary Science, 20(11), 691–737.
toggle visibility
Straub, A. (2007). An intelligent crow beats a lab. Science, 316(5825), 688.
toggle visibility
Legare, C. H., & Nielsen, M. (). Imitation and Innovation: The Dual Engines of Cultural Learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(11), 688–699.
toggle visibility
Milgram, N. W., Head, E., Muggenburg, B., Holowachuk, D., Murphey, H., Estrada, J., et al. (2002). Landmark discrimination learning in the dog: effects of age, an antioxidant fortified food, and cognitive strategy. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 26(6), 679–695.
toggle visibility
Peham, C., Licka, T., Schobesberger, H., & Meschan, E. (2004). Influence of the rider on the variability of the equine gait. European Workshop on Movement Science, 23(5), 663–671.
toggle visibility
Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print