toggle visibility Search & Display Options

Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print
Paz-y-Miño C. G., Bond, A. B., Kamil, A. C., & Balda, R. P. (2004). Pinyon jays use transitive inference to predict social dominance. Nature, 430(7001), 778–781.
toggle visibility
Barton, N. (1998). Evolutionary biology: The geometry of adaptation. Nature, 395(6704), 751–752.
toggle visibility
Whiten, A., Horner, V., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2005). Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees. Nature, 437(7059), 737–740.
toggle visibility
Shettleworth, S. J. (2004). Cognitive science: rank inferred by reason. Nature, 430(7001), 732–733.
toggle visibility
Fedak Ma, S. H. (1979). Reappraisal of energetics of locomotion shows identical cost in bipeds and quadrupeds including ostrich and horse. Nature, 282, 713–716.
toggle visibility
Foster, K. R., & Ratnieks, F. L. W. (2000). Social insects: Facultative worker policing in a wasp. Nature, 407(6805), 692–693.
toggle visibility
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., et al. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399(6737), 682–685.
toggle visibility
Parr, L. A., & de Waal, F. B. (1999). Visual kin recognition in chimpanzees (Vol. 399).
toggle visibility
de Waal, F. B. (1999). Cultural primatology comes of age. Nature, 399(6737), 635–636.
toggle visibility
Harrison, S. A., & Tong, F. (2009). Decoding reveals the contents of visual working memory in early visual areas. Nature, 458(7238), 632–635.
toggle visibility
Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print