toggle visibility Search & Display Options

Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print
Crook, J. H. (1983). On attributing consciousness to animals. Nature, 303(5912), 11–14.
toggle visibility
Dyer, F. C. (1998). Spatial Cognition: Lessons from Central-place Foraging Insects. In Russell P. Balda, Irene M. Pepperberg, & Alan C. Kamil (Eds.), Animal Cognition in Nature (pp. 119–154). London: Academic Press.
toggle visibility
Smith, W. J. (1998). Cognitive Implications of an Information-sharing Model of Animal Communication. In Russell P. Balda, Irene M. Pepperberg, & Alan C. Kamil (Eds.), Animal Cognition in Nature (pp. 227–243). London: Academic Press.
toggle visibility
Beer, C. G. (1998). Varying Views of Animal and Human Cognition. In Russell P. Balda, Irene M. Pepperberg, & Alan C. Kamil (Eds.), Animal Cognition in Nature (pp. 435–456). London: Academic Press.
toggle visibility
Goodall J. (1964). Tool-using and aimed throwing in a community of free-living chimpanzees. Nature, 201, 1264.
toggle visibility
Sugiyama Y. (1994). Tool use by wild chimpanzees. Nature, 376, 327.
toggle visibility
Gilmanshin, R., Callender, R. H., & Dyer, R. B. (1998). The core of apomyoglobin E-form folds at the diffusion limit. Nat Struct Biol, 5(5), 363–365.
toggle visibility
de Waal, F. B. (1999). Cultural primatology comes of age. Nature, 399(6737), 635–636.
toggle visibility
Shettleworth, S. J. (2007). Animal behaviour: planning for breakfast. Nature, 445(7130), 825–826.
toggle visibility
Shettleworth, S. J. (2004). Cognitive science: rank inferred by reason. Nature, 430(7001), 732–733.
toggle visibility
Select All    Deselect All
 | 
Citations
 | 
   print